Zero power and selflessness: What meditation and conscious perception have
in common
Sean O Nuallain
Published in cognitive
Sciences 4(2)
University of Ireland, Ca and
and Nous Research, Dublin, Ireland
president@universityofireland.com
Abstract
This paper attempts to reconstrue the art and science
of meditation in the context of an overall theory of cognition, and with
reference to evidence from simulated and real data analysed in a neurodynamical
framework. First, we discuss the phenomenology of meditation and its relation
to the known evidence. It is argued that meditation is on a continuum with the
types of conscious mental activity characterized by gamma waves, particularly
synchronized gamma. Specifically, it is suggested that gamma synchrony in
meditation allows the normally prominent background noise of the brain
momentarily to subside in like synchrony. Secondly, a set of experiments using
both simulated and real data and interpreted in a neurodynamical context that
bear on the issue of meditation is described. The isomorphism between the
similar and real data is established before a set of ECOG data involving a
single brain in epileptic seizure, resting and performing a task is adduced. It
is argued that seizure, above all,
reveals the desirability of intervals of “zero power”. Thirdly, the
theoretical and experimental frameworks are brought together into a brief
sketch of an overall perspective that impacts on cognition as on applied
experientialism.
1. Introduction
The neurophysiological literature on meditation is
vast and confusing. It ranges from the borders of the New Age (Murphy et al,
1988) to more hardcore science (Wallace et al, 1972) to the hallowed PNAS (Lutz
et al, 2004). The benefits of meditation
have plausibly been proven to be perceptual (Tloczynski et al, 2000),
anti-ageing due to gross cortical changes (Lazar et al, 2005), and finally
enormous in that links between the nervous and immune systems are exploited to
implement improvements in health (Davidson et al, 2003). One recent paper has
implicitly challenged the consensus that gamma synchrony is at the core of the
successful meditative experience; yet Chang et al's (2005) paper shows no sign of
having tested for gamma, which they do not even explicitly mention. In their
paper, it is handled, if at all, as high beta, and in fact can be encompassed
within the theoretical framework about to be outlined here.
Perhaps a more important contrarian voice, from an
anthology of his writings on the subject,
is that of J. Krishnamurti (1999);
“Meditation is one of the greatest arts in life.....it
has no technique, and therefore no authority” (P. 2)
“Meditation according to a system is the avoidance of
the fact of what you are; it is far more important to understand yourself” (p.
55)
Following classical practical spirituality (described,
inter alia, in Ó Nualláin (2006)), Krishnamurti emphasises self-observation as
the key to that experience he calls “meditation”;
“Every system, method, binds thought to time, but
choiceless awareness of every thought and feeling.....is the beginning of
meditation” (41)
“Meditation is not something different from daily
life.....Watching all that, realizing your part in it, all that is part of
meditation” (12)
The enemy, again as in classical spirituality, is what
is normally known as the “self”
“Without knowing the activities of the self,
meditation becomes sensuous excitement and therefore of very little significance”(7)
“In that attention there is no frontier, there is no
center, as the 'me' who is aware or attentive. That attention, that silence, is
a state of meditation” (21)
While disabusing the initiate of the notion that there
will be spectacular visions, Krishnamurti
is eager to emphasise that the experience of the senses will be sharpened and
heightened ;
“Perception without the word .....is more acute,not
only with the brain but also with all the senses. ....It can be called a total
perception, and is part of meditation” (25)
So we now have an echo of Tloczynski et al (2000) in
that sense-perception is made more acute as a result of meditation. However,
much more can be said
“As you watched, a great stillness came into you. The
brain itself became very still....the quality of that silence, that stillness,
is not felt by the brain; it is beyond the brain” (63-64)
“Meditation which began at unknown depths, and went on
with increasing intensity and sweep, carved the brain into total silence,
scooping out the depths of thought, uprooting feeling” (79)
Again, this must be in some sense an egoless state,
with “thought” and the empirical self now explicitly being identified;
“And it was meditation without the meditator. The
meditator interferes with his stupidities and vanities, ambitions and greed. The meditator is
thought” (ibid.)
Finally, Krishnamurti (4) presages much of the
discussion about to take place with one comment;
“Meditation is to find out whether the brain, with all
its activities, all its experiences, can be absolutely quiet...that quietness
is not sleep but is tremendously active and therefore quiet.... A big dynamo
that is working perfectly hardly makes a sound; it is only when there is
friction that there is noise”
Let us anticipate the major argument in this paper. By
using the Hilbert transform, we can calculate, moment to moment, the power of
the background activity of the brain, which itself is a major consumer of the
total energy of the entire organism. When superimposed over by any oscillation,
the phenomena of “beats” occurs and the power of the background activity is
momentarily zero. The faster the superimposing wave, the more frequent the
occurrence of zero power (and thus amplitude, which explains the apparently
anomalous findings of Chang et al (2005)). So gamma oscillations will cause the
greatest number of zero power moments. Obviously, while totally synchronized
gamma is observationally impossible (given the relative crudity of the measures
like calcium ions that are used), the impressively synchronized observations of
Lutz et al (2004) will produce many moments of zero power. So the observations
of Chang et al and Lutz et al can be synthesised; high gamma leads to a
frequent incidence of zero power moments.
Elsewhere (O Nualláin, 2008, and forthcoming) , this
author has argued that self is essentially an artefact of the necessary
maintenance of differentiation between subject and object. In the safe environments in which meditation happens,
these distinctions can, at least momentarily, be let go. It is possible that,
through Buddhist and other meditative training, the self can be identified with
observation itself, with choiceless awareness. Not only that; the brain is now
maximally sensitive (Freeman 2002, 2003, 2005a and b).
By the end of this paper, the reader will hopefully be
convinced of the plausibility of this scenario vis a vis “formal” Buddhist
meditation. However, we have seen Krishnamurti argue that this type of ritual
is unnecessary; meditation starts with self-observation, and can be experienced
anywhere. Moreover, there is an extremely well-attested body of work by Singer
et al (1995) indicating that beta and gamma synchrony accompany conscious
perception. Can we weave together a narrative in which all these strands
cohere?
With respect to such a narrative,Singer (2007) emphasises
temporal codes in the brain, and argues that gamma synchrony is ubiquitous in
cognition. Elsewhere (O
Nualláin et al, in press) our group at Nous Research has produced a theoretical
model that can explain how an oscillation like gamma can affect the firing of
individual neurons. For the central purposes of the current paper, the recent
work by Melloni, Singer et al (2007) is critical, wherein it is showed that
conscious perception depends on prior gamma synchrony
Consequently, both
meditation and conscious perception seem intimately related to gamma activity.
After perusing section 2 below, the reader will hopefully be open to the argument
that the major role of gamma, and in particular gamma that is synchronized
and/or high amplitude, may well be its capacity to induce moments of zero power
on the overall activity of the brain.
On a phenomenal level,
there is a consensus that the meditative state is in some way selfless ( Ó Nualláin,
2006). Gazzaniga (2008) summarises his life's work by re-emphasising the theme
that our experience has a narrative imposed on it by an “interpreter” in the
left hemisphere. This is consistent with the famous Libet (1994) observation
that the action potential for a voluntary act has been in preparation for fully
tenths of seconds before we become aware of it. Our narratives to ourselves
impose continuity on discontinuity, and attribute agency to ourselves when
utterly inappropriate. Agency as a concept needs to be correspondingly
attenuated; when the intending of an act presents itself to consciousness, it
is experienced as a cause; consciousness of the consequences thereof are
experienced as effects. This vastly consequential idea is consistent with Libet's classic work and amplified in the
Freeman opera (ibid.).
Once an action is lined
up, the brain prepares the system for the sensory consequences of this action
in what is called the preafference process. According to Freeman, globally
coherent brain activity may be an objective correlate of consciousness through
preafference. Preafference, in turn,
enters once the more veridical notion of circular causality is substituted for
the stimulus-response act. The
consequences for consciousness qua process are enormous. It functions similarly
to an organizer in a thermostat. Hume was right; there is no conscious will,
but there does exist a conscious “won't”.
Meditation occurs in contexts in which the organism is
expecting minimal sensory consequences of its actions. If it is the case that regular states of
“Zero power” and maximum sensitivity occur, the self may well be able to
identify with and thus make a phenomenal state of that zero power process.
Consciousness may be due either to the globally coherent activity, or to the
fact that the brain has entered a zero power state through which consciousness
can now transparently act. This issue is
possibly unresolvable even in principle;
we explore it a little more in conclusion.
2. Experimental evidence
Freeman,
O'Nuallain and Rodriguez(in press) include much of the detail of the work about
to be presented here. O Nuallain (in
preparation)specifies some of the programming detail left out in that paper.
In summary, phase slippage and beat phenomena are
ubiquitous in any form of wave superposition. Likewise, it is trivially true
that cumulative summation of individual random events gives rise to Brownian
motion simulation. Freeman et al (ibid.) proposes a simulation environment in
which these phenomena are interrelated with cortical function. In particular,
we have found that phase slippage for brown noise filtered at frequencies
corresponding to beta and gamma EEG gives null power spike effects. These null
spikes, in turn, occur at rates similar to those that we have experimentally
found in test subjects. We compared data from our simulation with those taken
from a human experimental subject undergoing neurosurgery for epileptic
seizure; the protocol is described in Freeman (2006).
2.1 Methodology
We begin from the assumption that neuronal activity in
the resting brain can be simulated by
white noise. Specifically, the average firing and refractory periods suggest a
Nyquist frequency of 250 Hz, with a nominal timestep of 2 ms. There is
therefore nothing remarkable about the white noise, as indicated in Figure 1a.
The facts that true randomness cannot of course be algorithmically generated,
and that there would be numerical, as distinct from mathematical inexactitude,
had some consequences in that the null
spikes we describe below occasionally did not in fact go to zero.
Figure 1a; pseudo-white noise, simulating random firing of
neurons with the brain in a resting state.
White noise is simply a
sum over excitatory neurons, stabilized by refractory periods rather than
inhibition. Rather, inhibition provides a bandpass filter. In the case where we
have a short delay and feedback, gamma oscillations ensue; for longer such, it
is beta. “Real” white noise, as represented in 1b, shows internal correlation,
as would be expected;
We then considered
populations of neurons, which we terms the “mesoscopic” level of analysis
(Freeman 2000). This, again, is
construed as a cumulative summation of the white noise. Alternatively, of
course, we could have chosen to sum the later-applied filter directly to the
white noise; our investigation in this direction indicated that the results
were exactly the same. The frequency
profile was as suggested in Schroeder(1991).
2.2 Findings
Freeman (1975, 2002, 2003)
provides the interpretive context for this work. In a neurodynamics approach, he argues that 3-7
times a second, the brain approaches a limit attractor cycle at which any
external signal is amplified as the noise abates at the singularity. Consequently, there is no need to invoke stochastic
resonance to explain how extremely weak signals get detected. At this point,
the phase is undefined and background noise is
minimised. Freeman (2006) features experimental data showing this phase
slippage occurring in experimental subjects at the intervals predicted.
Let us first look at the
log of analytic power plotted against time at both the theta and gamma frequencies (Figs 2a and 2b)
for the simulated data. Observations of
EEG show frame rates in the range of 3-7 Hz; at the low end for beta, and the
high end for gamma. The results here support this, and below we have a
comparison with real data. Freeman (2005a) has suggested that the cortex can
change with great rapidity from one complex state into another since it has
effectively got a fractal structure. Self-similarity is therefore a crucial
theme; our work shows that the analysis of the brownian motion at various
filters shows similarity in the graph form
Figure 2a; Log of analytic
power graphed against time, theta frequency.
Figure 2b; Log of analytic
power graphed against time, gamma frequency.
We include a figure
(Figure 3a)showing that the power is occasionally less during the null spikes
than the peak by a factor of up to ten thousand; this is encouragingly similar
in the “real data” version in Figure 3b
Figures 3a and 3b
Under analysis, the real
data showed almost identical null spiking behaviour, as indicated in figures 4a
and 4b. As the data were collected under a different regime, and in order to
demonstrate the generality of the method, we use a different Nyquist frequency
for the simulated data
Figure 4a null spike
pattern, gamma frequency, simulated data
Figure 4b Null spike
pattern, gamma frequency, real data
However, the gross power consumption of the brain is the
issue that we are most concerned with here. The hypothesis is that, by gamma
wave superposition on the background noise, which “noise” actually comprises a
significant proportion of the organism's metabolic energy, meditation and
indeed fully conscious perception
results in less energy consumption by the organism. Figure 5a shows one extreme
case, that of brown noise during an epileptic seizure, where the lack of
stabilization by refractory periods results in massive metabolic requirements.
It is worth pointing out that, although the negative exponential suggested by
5b does not always occur in the case of the resting brain, all data sets
involving seizure show this monotonic upward trend;
This can be contrasted with the metabolic requirement of a brain at rest in 5b, which conforms with the classical Freeman requirement of a negative exponent, to which he attributes the self-similarity in 2a and 2b;
For the removal of doubt, it should be said that the scaling is exactly the same in each. Finally, 5c shows the same spectrum for a brain doing a cognitive task;
2.3 Interpretation of the
experimental evidence
The general hypothesis
that there exists periods of self-organised criticality in which brain activity
is essentially “reset” is supported by this work. Moreover, it can indeed be
argued that this vital aspect of brain activity is a result of a general
mathematical property of the filtering
of brown noise at certain fractions of the Nyquist frequency. A narrative
emerges; beta and gamma are artifacts of the operation of inhibitory neurons.
These frequencies, superimposed on the brown noise provided by the mass action
of groups of neurons, introduce a “shutter” in the brain's operations. Just as
we need microsaccades in order to see anything, it seems to be the case that we need null spikes in activity for
cognition to continue. We intend to explore this aspect in future papers.
We are happy to make the
code and data available to any researchers who want to replicate the work.
There is one specific finding that we find intriguing, and may act as a
motivating factor. Can it be the case that brown noise filtered at frequencies
corresponding in Nyquist terms to gamma produces phase slips in the upper end
of the 3-7 times per second that we have observed as the canonical rate of
phase transition in the brain? If that is the case, the brain is exploiting a
general mathematical law in order to maximise its sensitivity at a certain
predetermined rate (which, again intriguingly, is of the same order as the
“conscious moment” of a tenth of a second, the minimum time from which we can
consciously access a perceived scene)
Buttressing this idea, the
convergence of the behaviour of our simulation with real data is a helpful
result. We intend, in collaboration with
others, to extend the analysis to rabbit data from the visual and auditory
cortices that were gathered at a Nyquist of 250. That will allow attestation of
the notion that the brain maximises its sensitivity by exploiting the filtering
of brown noise at certain frequencies as well as grounding in real experimental
data.
3. Pansychism or internal
process? - some final speculation on consciousness
What is being
asserted, then, is that conscious states
comprise a sparse sample of the wave packets propagating through the brain
(Freeman, 2002) that embody motor commands, corollary discharges, and
pre-perceptions that we conceive as unconscious. The idea, pioneered by Piaget (1954, P. 434)
that “thought in its various aspects
reproduces on its own plane the processes....in the case of sensorimotor
intelligence and the structure of the initial practical universe” is accepted.
Wave packets embodying motor commands are the substratum for mathematical and
other abstract thought. Furthermore, focal consciousness samples at far too
slow a rate to give veridical access to the contents of our cortices, and
nature has gifted us various mechanisms to get around this, some of which are
mentioned in my 2008 paper.
The meditative context inevitably facilitates attenuation of
wave packet behaviour; it is normally a safe, stimulation-poor environment.
Meditators are encouraged not to think of worldly concerns; even better, to
simply observe such thoughts rising and falling. The attested health benefits
of meditation may be partly due to the context itself -
after all, it is quite a boon to be free of worries sufficiently as to
be able just to sit for a while - and,
intriguingly, the fact that the power use of the brain will be lessened for a
while, freeing metabolic energy up for such matters as getting glucose into
cells.
However, many a reader will feel cheated by what looks like
a philistinical exercise. What of the feelings of blessedness that even Chang
et al (2005) speak of? Let us return to the issue of the self in meditation.
From the Advaita Vedanta context which Krishnamurti has brilliantly popularised
in the West, the self is conscious awareness. It is also identical with
Brahman, reality, consciousness, being, bliss. It will always be open to
scientist to reduce the meditative experience to the brain process that induces
it as it well be open to the meditator to claim that her experience as a result
of this process puts her in touch with a further experience of self which
transcends everyday experience. It is this writer's guess that this final
observation is the most certain statement in this largely technical paper.
References
Chang,
Kanf-Ming and Lo, Pei-chan (2005).
Meditation EEG Interpretation based on novel fuzzy-merging strategies and
wavelet features Biomedical engineering Appl Basis Comm 167-17
Davidson, R.
J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S.
F., Urbanowski, F., Harrington, A., Bonus, K. & Sheridan, J. F.
“Alterations in Brain and Immune Function Produced by Mindfulness
Meditation”(2003) Psychosom. Med. 65, 564-570
Freeman WJ
[1975] Mass Action in the Nervous System. New York: Academic.
Electronic
2004: http://sulcus.berkeley.edu/MANSWWW/MANSWWW.html
Freeman WJ
[2000] Neurodynamics: An Exploration of Mesoscopic Brain
Dynamics, London UK:
Springer
Freeman, W. (2002) “How
and why brains create meaning from sensory information” International journal
of bifurcation and chaos, Vol 14, No 2 (2004), 515-530
Freeman, W. (2003) “A
neurobiological theory of meaning in perception Part 1” International journal
of bifurcation and chaos, Vol. 13. No. 9 2493-2511
Freeman WJ [2005a] A
field-theoretic approach to understanding scale-free neocortical dynamics
Biological Cybernetics 2005, 92/6: 350-359
Freeman WJ [2005b] William
James on Consciousness, revisited. Chaos and Complexity letters, 1 (1) : 17-43
Freeman
WJ [2006 ] Origin, structure, and role of background EEG activity. Part 4.
Neural frame simulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117: 572-589.
Freeman,
W., S. O'Nuallain and J Rodriguez(in press) "Simulating cortical
background electrocortigram at rest with filtered noise" Journal of
integrated neuroscience, Sept 2008
Gazzaniga,
M (2008) Human NY;Ecco/Harper Collins
Krishnamurti,
J. (1999) Meditations Boston: Shambala
Lazar,
Sara W.; Kerr, Catherine E.; Wasserman, Rachel H.; Gray, Jeremy R. ; Greve, Douglas
N.; Treadway, Michael T.; McGarvey, Metta ; Quinn, Brian T.; Dusek, Jeffery A.;
Benson, Herbert ; Rauch, Scott L.; Moore, Christopher I.; Fischl, Bruce (2005)”Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. ” Neuroreport.
16(17):1893-1897, November 28, 2005.
Libet B [1994] Neurophysiology of Consciousness: Selected Papers and New
Essays. Boston MA: Birkhauser.
Lutz,
A., Lawrence L. Greischar, Nancy B. Rawlings, Mathieu Ricard and Richard
J.Davidson(2004)“Long-term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma
synchrony during mental practice, The
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 101(46)16369-16373
Melloni,
L., Molina, C., Pena, M., Torres, D., Singer, W., and Rodriguez, E.
Synchronization of neural activity across cortical areas correlates with
conscious perception. J. Neurosci. 2007; ; 27: 2858-2865
Murphy,M.,Donovan.
S (1988)"A Bibliography of Meditation Theory and Research:
1931-1983."
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology 15, no. 2 : 181-228.
The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology 15, no. 2 : 181-228.
Ó Nualláin, Seán (2003) The Search for mind;
third edition. Exeter: England
Ó Nualláin, Seán (2004) Being Human: The Search for order;
second edition. Exeter: England
Ó Nualláin, Seán(2006). Inner and outer empiricism in
consciousness research. New Ideas in Psychology 24 (1):30-40
Ó Nualláin, Seán,
Paul Mc Kevitt and Eoghan Mac Aogain (eds.) (1997)"Two Sciences of
Mind" (Benjamins)
O
Nualláin, Seán (2008)“Subjects and Objects” Biosemiotics journal, Volume 2, Pp.
239-251
O
Nualláin, Seán (forthcoming)“Neural correlates of consciousness of what?” New
ideas in psychology
O
Nualláin, Seán and T. Doris (in press)
“What is neural resonance for?” Chaos and complexity letters 3(4)
O
Nuallain (in preparation) “The Hilbert transform applied to ECOG data”
Piaget (1954) The Construction of Reality in the
child. New York:
Ballantine
Raichle, ME (2006) “The brain's dark
energy” Science 314: 1249-1250
Schroeder, M. (1991)
Fractals, chaos, power laws. New York;
WH Freeman
Singer, W (2007) "Binding by synchrony"
Scholarpedia, 2(12):1657
Singer W, Gray CM (1995). Visual feature
integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annu Rev Neurosci 18:
555–586.
Tloczynski, Joseph, et
al., "Perception of Visual Illusions by Novice and Longer-Term
Meditators". Perceptual and Motor Skills 2000: 1021-1027.
Wallace, R.K. &
Benson, H. (1972)The Physiology of Meditation. Scientific American,
226(2):84-90